Related article: knees and lowered themselves onto it. Some simply perched on top,
others sat down so hard that the chair skittered out from under them.
Some children sat on the miniature slide and tried to ride down Almond Cucumber Peel-Off Mask it,
usually falling off in the process; others attempted to climb the
steps, causing the slide to tip over. (With the chair and slide made
of sturdy plastic and only about five inches tall, the toddlers faced
no danger of hurting themselves.) A few kids tried to get into the
tiny car. Just as they had done with the large version, they opened
the door and attempted--often with remarkable persistence--to force a
foot inside. One little girl went so far as to take off her shoe in
the apparent hope that her foot would then fit!
Interestingly, most of the children showed little or no reaction to
their failed attempts with the miniatures. A couple seemed a bit
angry, a few looked sheepish, but most simply went on to do something
else. We think the lack of reaction probably reflects the fact that
toddlers' daily lives are full of unsuccessful attempts to do one
thing or another.
Our interpretation is that scale errors originate in a dissociation
between the use of visual information for planning an action and for
controlling its execution. When a child sees a miniature of a familiar
object, visual information--the object's shape, color, texture and so
on--activates the child's mental representation of its referent.
Associated with that memory is the motor program for interacting with
the large object and other similar objects. In half the children we
studied, this motor program was presumably activated but then
inhibited, and the children did not attempt to interact with the
miniature in the same way as they did with the large version.
But in the other half the motor routine was not inhibited. Once the
child began to carry out the typical motor sequence, visual
information about the actual size of the object was used to accurately
perform the actions. Some children, for instance, bent over the tiny
chair and looked between their legs to precisely locate it; those
trying to get into the miniature car first opened its door and then
tried to shove their foot right in. In deciding to interact with the
replica, the children relied on visual information linking it to the
normal-size object, but in executing their plan, they used visual
information about the miniature's actual size to guide their actions.
This dissociation in the use of visual information is consistent with
influential theories of visual processing--ones positing that
different regions of the brain handle object recognition and planning
versus the execution and control of actions.
Scale errors involve a failure of dual representation: children cannot
maintain the distinction between a symbol and its referent. We know
this because the confusion between referent and symbolic object does
not happen when the demand for dual representation is eliminated--a
discovery I made in 1997 when Rosengren and Kevin F. Miller of the
University of Illinois and I convinced two-and-a-half-year-olds--with
the full consent of their parents, of course--that we had a device
that could miniaturize everyday objects.
The Magical Machine
Using our amazing shrinking machine, we hoped to see if the need to
think of an object in two ways at once was at the heart of young
children's inability to appreciate symbols. If a child believes that a
machine has shrunk an object or a room, then in the child's mind the
miniature is the thing itself. There is no symbolic relation between
room and model, so children should be able to apply what they know
about the big version to the little one.
We used the powers of our device to turn toys into miniature versions
of themselves and to shrink a large tent. In front of the child, we
placed a toy--a troll doll with vivid purple hair--in a tent and aimed
the shrinking machine at the tent. The child and experimenter then
decamped to another room to wait while the machine did its work. When
they returned to the lab, a small tent sat where the big one had been.
(One of the remarkable things about this study is the fact that the
children did not find it at all surprising that a machine could
miniaturize objects. Or that it might need privacy to do so.)
When we asked the children to search for the toy, they immediately